Skip to content Skip to navigation

Study finds that the benefits of psychological interventions don't end with the individual but can trigger change in a group

Stanford study finds that the benefits of psychological interventions don't end with the individual but can trigger change in a group. (Photo: michaeljung/Shutterstock)
Stanford study finds that the benefits of psychological interventions don't end with the individual but can trigger change in a group. (Photo: michaeljung/Shutterstock)
Joseph Powers (Photo: Marc Franklin)
Joseph Powers (Photo: Marc Franklin)
Geoffrey Cohen
Geoffrey Cohen

Study finds that the benefits of psychological interventions don't end with the individual but can trigger change in a group

How shielding a few students from stereotypes benefits everyone.

Sharing a classroom with students who are protected from negative stereotypes about their group can boost all students' grades, new Stanford research shows.

The study was published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science. Joseph Powers, a Stanford psychology graduate student and lead author on the paper, said the research showed that sharing a classroom with a few students who are protected from negative stereotypes can help the entire class perform better gradewise.

"Changing the psychological processes of just a few individuals can transform the larger group in ways that benefit everyone," said Powers, who was joined in the research by Stanford psychology Professor Geoffrey Cohen. Both are at Stanford Graduate School of Education.

The researchers essentially protected African American students from "stereotype threats" through subtle psychological interventions. A stereotype threat is a situation in which people feel at risk of confirming negative stereotypes about their group, ethnic or social. This, for example, can lead to minority students underperforming academically as compared with white students. Researchers have identified and documented over the last two decades that this phenomenon does exist.

Overcoming deep-rooted stereotypes is not easy, so Powers and Cohen sought to examine whether "values affirmation" interventions that encourage a few minority students in a classroom to focus on their best qualities can help the other students improve grades, too. One common way to affirm values is through written exercises – the key is that students reflect on and are conscious of the things that matter the most to them personally.

The Stanford project consisted of studies involving seventh-grade students from middle- to lower-middle-class families at a suburban northeastern middle school. All ethnic groups were studied. During the intervention, students received sealed envelopes with writing materials from their teachers. They then wrote about what personal attributes they valued the most.

After about 15 minutes of writing, students placed the materials back in the envelopes, and the materials were returned to the researchers. Students believed they were participating in a regular classroom writing assignment – they did not know it was part of a research study. The grades of the students receiving the intervention and those who did not were later compared.

Individuals impact the group

The findings showed that the classrooms with higher concentrations of African American students protected from stereotype threat by the intervention triggered higher academic performances among all classmates – regardless of race or participation in the intervention. This effect was most pronounced among students with a history of poor performance.

The data indicate that adding just two African American students to the intervention group in a typical classroom improved the classroom environment enough that low-performing students' grades increased, on average, by a third of a letter grade from a C to a C+.

"Results suggest that the benefits of psychological intervention do not end with the individual. Changed individuals can improve their social environments, and such improvements can benefit others regardless of whether they participated in the intervention," Powers and Cohen wrote.

The analysis confirmed that this overall classroom improvement effect was not influenced by factors such as student race or the teachers involved.

The intervention triggered not only a change in individuals, but also a change in the group atmosphere. Previous research documented how the intervention improved academic performance and social belonging among African American students, but the new studies found that classrooms with higher concentrations of these students were triggering higher performance among all classmates, regardless of race or participation in the intervention.

Powers said, "The main implication is that we may often underestimate the full effect of social programs and interventions if we only measure for direct effects among participants. The concentration of beneficiaries within a group can trigger effects as large or larger than the initial program or intervention."

Powers suggested that future research should explore the precise mechanisms that allow or inhibit such reactions. For example, does a classroom with a higher concentration of students receiving affirmations of their core values create new ways of understanding achievement – and does this also help free up resources by empowering teachers to do their job in a psychologically safer environment?

Cohen has found in earlier research that with African American students, affirmations can improve academic performance at the individual level by reducing stress over being labeled with a negative stereotype.

Other co-authors on the paper included Julio Garcia, also a researcher at Stanford, Jonathan Cook, an assistant professor of psychology from Pennsylvania State University; Valerie Purdie-Vaughns, an associate professor of psychology from Columbia University; and Nancy Apfel, a researcher from Yale University.


Get the Educator

Subscribe to our monthly newsletter.

Back to the Top